Mirroring People: The New Science of How We Connect with Others

Mirroring People: The New Science of How We Connect with Others

by Marco Iacoboni
Mirroring People: The New Science of How We Connect with Others

Mirroring People: The New Science of How We Connect with Others

by Marco Iacoboni

eBookFirst Edition (First Edition)

$11.99 

Available on Compatible NOOK Devices and the free NOOK Apps.
WANT A NOOK?  Explore Now

Related collections and offers


Overview

What accounts for the remarkable ability to get inside another person's head—to know what they're thinking and feeling? "Mind reading" is the very heart of what it means to be human, creating a bridge between self and others that is fundamental to the development of culture and society. But until recently, scientists didn't understand what in the brain makes it possible.

This has all changed in the last decade. Marco Iacoboni, a leading neuroscientist whose work has been covered in The New York Times, the Los Angeles Times, and The Wall Street Journal, explains the groundbreaking research into mirror neurons, the "smart cells" in our brain that allow us to understand others. From imitation to morality, from learning to addiction, from political affiliations to consumer choices, mirror neurons seem to have properties that are relevant to all these aspects of social cognition. As The New York Times reports: "The discovery is shaking up numerous scientific disciplines, shifting the understanding of culture, empathy, philosophy, language, imitation, autism and psychotherapy."

Mirroring People is the first book for the general reader on this revolutionary new science.


Product Details

ISBN-13: 9781429990752
Publisher: Farrar, Straus and Giroux
Publication date: 06/23/2009
Sold by: Macmillan
Format: eBook
Pages: 320
File size: 371 KB

About the Author

Marco Iacoboni is a neurologist and neuroscientist at the David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA. He has appeared on Good Morning America, the Early Show, and Morning Edition, among other TV and radio programs.


Marco Iacoboni is a neurologist and neuroscientist at the David Geffen School of Medicine at UCLA. His research has been covered by newspapers around the country, and he has appeared on ABC Good Morning America, the CBS Early Show, and NPR Morning Edition, among other TV and radio programs. He is the author of Mirroring People.

Read an Excerpt

CHAPTER 1

Monkey See, Monkey Do

NEURO THIS!

When we get right down to it, what do we human beings do all day long? We read the world, especially the people we encounter. My face in the mirror first thing in the morning doesn't look too good, but the face beside me in the mirror tells me that my lovely wife is off to a good start. One glance at my eleven-year-old daughter at the breakfast table tells me to tread carefully and sip my espresso in silence. When a colleague reaches for a wrench in the laboratory, I know he's going to work on the magnetic stimulation machine, and he's not going to throw his tool against the wall in anger. When another colleague walks in with a grin or a smirk on her face — the line can be fine indeed, the product of tiny differences in the way we set our face muscles — I automatically and almost instantaneously can discern which it is. We all make dozens — hundreds — of such distinctions every day. It is, quite literally, what we do.

Nor do we give any of this a second thought. It all seems so ordinary. However, it is actually extraordinary — and extraordinary that it feels ordinary! For centuries, philosophers scratched their heads over humans' ability to understand one another. Their befuddlement was reasonable: they had essentially no science to work with. For the past 150 years or so, psychologists, cognitive scientists, and neuroscientists have had some science to work with — and in the past fifty years, a lot of science — and for a long time they continued to scratch their heads. No one could begin to explain how it is that we know what others are doing, thinking, and feeling.

Now we can. We achieve our very subtle understanding of other people thanks to certain collections of special cells in the brain called mirror neurons. These are the tiny miracles that get us through the day. They are at the heart of how we navigate through our lives. They bind us with each other, mentally and emotionally.

Why do we give ourselves over to emotion during the carefully crafted, heartrending scenes in certain movies? Because mirror neurons in our brains re-create for us the distress we see on the screen. We have empathy for the fictional characters — we know how they're feeling — because we literally experience the same feelings ourselves. And when we watch the movie stars kiss on-screen? Some of the cells firing in our brain are the same ones that fire when we kiss our lovers. "Vicarious" is not a strong enough word to describe the effect of these mirror neurons. When we see someone else suffering or in pain, mirror neurons help us to read her or his facial expression and actually make us feel the suffering or the pain of the other person. These moments, I will argue, are the foundation of empathy and possibly of morality, a morality that is deeply rooted in our biology. Do you watch sports on television? If so, you must have noticed the many "reaction shots" in the stands: the fan frozen with anticipation, the fan ecstatic over the play. (This is especially true for baseball broadcasts, with all the downtime between pitches.) These shots are effective television because our mirror neurons make sure that by seeing these emotions, we share them. To see the athletes perform is to perform ourselves. Some of the same neurons that fire when we watch a player catch a ball also fire when we catch a ball ourselves. It is as if by watching, we are also playing the game. We understand the players' actions because we have a template in our brains for that action, a template based on our own movements. Since different actions share similar movement properties and activate similar muscles, we don't have to be skilled players to "mirror" the athletes in our brain. The mirror neurons of a non-tennis-playing fan will fire when watching a pro smash an overhead, because the non-tennis-playing fan has certainly made other kinds of overhead movements with his arm throughout his life; the equivalent neurons of a fan such as me, who also plays the game, will obviously be activated much more strongly. And if I'm watching Roger Federer, I bet my mirror neurons must be firing wildly, because I'm a big Federer fan.

Mirror neurons undoubtedly provide, for the first time in history, a plausible neurophysiological explanation for complex forms of social cognition and interaction. By helping us recognize the actions of other people, mirror neurons also help us to recognize and understand the deepest motives behind those actions, the intentions of other individuals. The empirical study of intention has always been considered almost impossible, because intentions were deemed too "mental" to be studied with empirical tools. How do we even know that other people have mental states similar to our own? Philosophers have mulled over this "problem of other minds" for centuries, with very little progress. Now they have some real science to work with. Research on mirror neurons gives them and everyone interested in how we understand one another some remarkable food for thought.

Consider the teacup experiment I dreamed up some years back, which I'll discuss in considerable detail later. The test subjects are shown three video clips involving the same simple action: a hand grasping a teacup. In one, there is no context for the action, just the hand and the cup. In another, the subjects see a messy table, complete with cookie crumbs and dirty napkins — the aftermath of a tea party, clearly. The third video shows a neatly organized tabletop, in apparent preparation for the tea party. In all three video clips, a hand reaches in to pick up the teacup. Nothing else happens, so the grasping action observed by the subjects in the experiment is exactly the same. The only difference is the context.

Do mirror neurons in the brains of our subjects note the difference in the contexts? Yes. When the subject is observing the grasping scene with no context at all, mirror neurons are the least active. They are more active when the subject is watching either of the scenes and most active when watching the neat scene. Why? Because drinking is a much more fundamental intention for us than is cleaning up. The teacup experiment is now well known in the field of neuroscience, but it is not an isolated result: solid empirical evidence suggests that our brains are capable of mirroring the deepest aspects of the minds of others — intention is definitely one such aspect — at the fine-grained level of a single brain cell. This is utterly remarkable. Equally remarkable is the effortlessness of this simulation. We do not have to draw complex inferences or run complicated algorithms. Instead, we use mirror neurons.

Looking at the issue from another perspective, labs around the world are accumulating evidence that social deficits, such as those associated with autism, may be due to a primary dysfunction of mirror neurons. I hypothesize that mirror neurons may also be very important in imitative violence induced by media violence, and we have preliminary evidence suggesting that mirror neurons are important in various forms of social identification, including "branding" and affiliation with a political party. Have you heard of neuroethics, neuromarketing, neuropolitics? You will in the years and decades to come, and research in these fields will be rooted, explicitly or otherwise, in the functions of mirror neurons.

This book tells the story of the serendipitous and groundbreaking discovery of this special class of brain cells, the remarkable advances in the field in just twenty years, and the extremely clever experiments now under way in several labs around the world. Quite simply, I believe this work will force us to rethink radically the deepest aspects of our social relations and our very selves. Some years ago, another researcher suggested that the discovery of mirror neurons promised to do for neuroscience what the discovery of DNA did for biology. That's an extraordinarily bold statement, because essentially everything in biology comes back to DNA. Decades in the future, will everything in neuroscience be seen as coming back to mirror neurons?

BRAIN SURPRISES

For fifteen years I have lived in Los Angeles and worked in my laboratory at UCLA, but as my name suggests, this story should rightly begin in Italy, and I'm happy to report that it really does — specifically, in the small and beautiful city of Parma, famous for its fabulous food, particularly prosciutto di Parma and Parmesan cheese, and for its music. Now we can add neuroscience to the list of Parma's world-class exports; it was at the university here that a group of neurophysiologists, led by my friend Giacomo Rizzolatti, first identified mirror neurons.

Rizzolatti and his colleagues work with Macaca nemestrina, a species of monkey often used in neuroscience labs worldwide. These macaque monkeys are very docile animals, unlike their more famous relatives, rhesus monkeys, who are highly competitive alpha-male types (even the females). Research on monkeys in a lab such as Rizzolatti's is predicated on its inferential value for understanding the human brain, which is generally considered the most complex entity in the known universe, with good reason. The human brain contains about one hundred billion neurons, each of which can make contact with thousands, even tens of thousands, of other neurons. These contacts, or synapses, are the means by which neurons communicate with one another, and their number is staggering. The distinguishing brain feature in mammals is the neocortex, the most recently evolved of our brain structures. Now here's the key "inferential" point: the macaque brain is only about one-fourth as large as ours, and our neocortex is much larger than the macaque's neocortex, but neuroanatomists typically agree that the structures in the neocortex of macaques and humans correspond relatively well despite these differences.

In Parma, the Rizzolatti team's pertinent area of study was an area of the brain labeled F5, located in a large region called the premotor cortex — that part of the neocortex concerned with planning, selecting, and executing actions. Area F5 contains millions of neurons that specialize in "coding" for one specific motor behavior: actions of the hand, including grasping, holding, tearing, and, most fundamental of all, bringing objects — food — to the mouth. For every macaque, as for every primate, these actions are as basic and essential as they come. We Homo sapiens are grasping and manipulating objects from the moment we fumble for the snooze button on the alarm clock until we adjust our pillows at bedtime, eighteen hours later. All in all, we each perform hundreds, if not thousands, of grasping actions every day. In fact, this is precisely why the Rizzolatti team chose area F5 for the closest possible investigation. All neuroscientists want to understand the brain for understanding's sake, but we also have an eye on more practical goals, such as discoveries that may eventually drive new treatments for disease. The discovery of the neurophysiological mechanisms of motor control of the hand in the macaque could eventually help individual humans with brain damage recover at least some degree of hand function.

Through laborious experimentation, the Rizzolatti team had acquired an impressive understanding of the actions of these motor cells during various "grasping" exercises with the monkeys. (They are called motor cells because they are the first in the sequence that controls the muscles that move the body.) Then one day, about twenty years ago, the neurophysiologist Vittorio Gallese was moving around the lab during a lull in the day's experiment. A monkey was sitting quietly in the chair, waiting for her next assignment. Suddenly, just as Vittorio reached for something — he does not remember what — he heard a burst of activity from the computer that was connected to the electrodes that had been surgically implanted in the monkey's brain. To the inexperienced ear, this activity would have sounded like static; to the ear of an expert neuroscientist, it signaled a discharge from the pertinent cell in area F5. Vittorio immediately thought the reaction was strange. The monkey was just sitting quietly, not intending to grasp anything, yet this neuron affiliated with the grasping action had fired nevertheless.

Or so goes one story about the first recorded observation of a mirror neuron. Another involves one of Vittorio's colleagues, Leo Fogassi, who picked up a peanut and triggered an excited response in F5. Yet another credits Vittorio Gallese and some ice cream. There are others, all plausible, none confirmed. Years later, when the importance of mirror neurons was clearly understood, the Parma colleagues went back to their lab notes, hoping to put together a fairly accurate timeline of their earliest observations, but they simply couldn't do it. They found references in their lab notes to "complex visual responses" of the monkeys' motor cells in area F5. Such notes were unclear, because the scientists did not know what to make of their observations at the time. Neither they nor any neuroscientist in the world could have imagined that motor cells could fire merely at the perception of somebody else's actions, with no motor action involved at all. In light of both knowledge and theory at the time, this made absolutely no sense. Cells in the monkey brain that send signals to other cells that are anatomically connected to muscles have no business firing when the monkey is completely still, hands in lap, watching somebody else's actions. And yet they did.

In the end, it does not matter much that the "Eureka!" moment for mirror neurons stretched over a period of years. What matters is that the team did soon grapple with the odd goings-on in their laboratory. They had a hard time believing these phenomena themselves, but in time they also sensed that the discovery, if confirmed, could be potentially groundbreaking. They were right. Twenty years after that first recording in the laboratory, a cascade of well-controlled experiments with monkeys and later with humans (different kinds of experiments, for the most part; no needles inserted through skulls) have confirmed the remarkable phenomenon. The simple fact that a subset of the cells in our brains — the mirror neurons — fire when an individual kicks a soccer ball, sees a ball being kicked, hears a ball being kicked, and even just says or hears the word "kick" leads to amazing consequences and new understandings.

THE FAB FOUR

We now know that about 20 percent of the cells in area F5 of the macaque brain are mirror neurons; 80 percent are not. Given those odds, the group in Parma was bound to come across mirror neurons sooner or later. When they did, the background assumptions not only of their lab but of neuroscientists around the globe were put to the test. In the 1980s, neuroscientists were deeply invested in the paradigm that held that the various functions implemented by the brain — macaque or human — were confined to separate boxes. Under this paradigm, perception (seeing objects, hearing sounds, and so on) and action (reaching for a piece of food, grasping it, putting it in the mouth) are entirely separate and independent of one another. A third function, cognition, is somehow "in between" perception and action and allows us to plan and select our motor behavior, to attend to specific things that are relevant to us, to disregard extraneous matters, to remember names and events, and so on. These three broadly construed functions were typically assumed to be separate in the brain. The paradigm reflected the justified bias in science for the most parsimonious explanation of phenomena. To dissect a complex phenomenon into simpler elements is a good principle for investigation. It is still the dominant approach in neurophysiology and neuroscience, and in many specialized areas of research it works well. For instance, researchers have identified neurons that respond only to horizontal lines in the visual field, while others code for vertical lines.

(Continues…)



Excerpted from "Mirroring People"
by .
Copyright © 2009 Marco Iacoboni.
Excerpted by permission of Picador.
All rights reserved. No part of this excerpt may be reproduced or reprinted without permission in writing from the publisher.
Excerpts are provided by Dial-A-Book Inc. solely for the personal use of visitors to this web site.

Table of Contents

Title Page,
ONE - Monkey See, Monkey Do,
TWO - Simon Says,
THREE - Grasping Language,
FOUR - See Me, Feel Me,
FIVE - Facing Yourself,
SIX - Broken Mirrors,
SEVEN - Super Mirrors and the Wired Brain,
EIGHT - The Bad and the Ugly: Violence and Drug Abuse,
NINE - Mirroring Wanting and Liking,
TEN - Neuropolitics,
ELEVEN - Existential Neuroscience and Society,
THE SCIENCE OF EMPATHY AND HOW WE CONNECT WITH OTHERS,
About the Author,
Afterword,
Notes,
Acknowledgments,
Index,
Copyright Page,

From the B&N Reads Blog

Customer Reviews