Do Animals Think?

Do Animals Think?

by Clive D. L. Wynne
ISBN-10:
0691126364
ISBN-13:
9780691126364
Pub. Date:
03/13/2006
Publisher:
Princeton University Press
ISBN-10:
0691126364
ISBN-13:
9780691126364
Pub. Date:
03/13/2006
Publisher:
Princeton University Press
Do Animals Think?

Do Animals Think?

by Clive D. L. Wynne
$32.0 Current price is , Original price is $32.0. You
$32.00 
  • SHIP THIS ITEM
    Qualifies for Free Shipping
  • PICK UP IN STORE
    Check Availability at Nearby Stores
  • SHIP THIS ITEM

    Temporarily Out of Stock Online

    Please check back later for updated availability.


Overview

Does your dog know when you've had a bad day? Can your cat tell that the coffee pot you left on might start a fire? Could a chimpanzee be trained to program your computer? In this provocative book, noted animal expert Clive Wynne debunks some commonly held notions about our furry friends. It may be romantic to ascribe human qualities to critters, he argues, but it's not very realistic. While animals are by no means dumb, they don't think the same way we do. Contrary to what many popular television shows would have us believe, animals have neither the "theory-of-mind" capabilities that humans have (that is, they are not conscious of what others are thinking) nor the capacity for higher-level reasoning. So, in Wynne's view, when Fido greets your arrival by nudging your leg, he's more apt to be asking for dinner than commiserating with your job stress.


That's not to say that animals don't possess remarkable abilities—and Do Animals Think? explores countless examples: there's the honeybee, which not only remembers where it found food but communicates this information to its hivemates through an elaborate dance. And how about the sonar-guided bat, which locates flying insects in the dark of night and devours lunch on the wing?


Engagingly written, Do Animals Think? takes aim at the work of such renowned animal rights advocates as Peter Singer and Jane Goodall for falsely humanizing animals. Far from impoverishing our view of the animal kingdom, however, it underscores how the world is richer for having such a diversity of minds—be they of the animal or human variety.


Product Details

ISBN-13: 9780691126364
Publisher: Princeton University Press
Publication date: 03/13/2006
Pages: 280
Product dimensions: 6.00(w) x 9.00(h) x (d)

About the Author

Clive D.L.Wynne is Associate Professor of Psychology at the University of Florida. He is the author of Animal Cognition: The Mental Lives of Animals

Read an Excerpt

Do Animals Think?


Chapter One

WHAT ARE ANIMALS?

I grew up on the Isle of Wight. "That southern island / Where the wild Tennyson became a fossil," W. H. Auden called it. A century ago it was very popular with poets. Today it is just a quaint English seaside resort: silent in the winter, bustling with tourists in the summer.

In 1994, long after I'd left on my travels, one Barry Horne woke up the sleepy Isle of Wight. He planted a succession of bombs. One in a pharmacy, another in a car parts store, a third in a fishing tackle supplier, and a fourth in a Cancer Research charity shop. The largest bomb caused nearly £3 million (about $5 million) of damage to the pharmacy. Horne chose that store because the parent company (Britain's largest pharmacy chain, Boots The Chemist) had in his view reneged on a promise to stop selling products tested on animals. The car parts store belonged to the same group as the pharmacy, so it had to go too. Fishing tackle-Horne was presumably objecting to angling. Cancer Research shops specialize in used clothing and household goods, sold by volunteers to raise money for research into cures for cancer. Where could be the objection to such good works? Cancer research involves animal experimentation, so the charity shop was a legitimate target also. He hid a bomb in a packet of cigarettes, which he stuffed into a leather bag. A Mrs. Woods bought the bag and took it away with her on a trip. Not knowing she was carrying a bomb, she let her children, aged three and six, play with her bag's contents. They found the fake pack of cigarettes. Fortunately, the device failed to explode.

I'm very puzzled by Horne. One thing I'd like to know is why he chose the Isle of Wight for his attacks. He was from Northampton, many miles away. Perhaps he had memories of a particularly miserable summer holiday on the beaches of the Isle of Wight. I resent that he chose to target a place so special to me.

But clearly for those of you not brought up on the Isle of Wight, the question is, Why would he plant bombs anywhere at all? Two things are worth noting here. The first is that there can be no doubting Horne's sincerity. In November 2001 he died of liver failure as a consequence of a hunger strike while serving an eighteen-year sentence for arson. He was trying to pressure the British government to set up an enquiry into animal experimentation.

The second important point is that Horne was not the British equivalent of the Unabomber (Theodore Kaczynski, a loner in Montana who directed bombs primarily at university personnel out of a deluded grudge against society). Horne was a member of the Animal Liberation Front-a group which refers to him on their web site as a "courageous fighter" with "thousands of supporters." It is difficult to gauge the degree of support for animal rights groups in the general population with any accuracy, but it is certainly not negligible: Horne's funeral was attended by three hundred people. So, though Horne obviously represents an extreme case, violent protest against what we do to animals is not limited to just one maverick.

I'm not trying to claim that all animal rights activists are vicious brutes, or that all the uses to which we put animals are defensible. What I am interested in is the range of attitudes in our society toward animals. Why, for example, given that Horne felt driven to wreak his vengeance on the Isle of Wight, did he choose to focus on shops? If he had to attack someone, why not the farmers on the hills? They do a lot more to animals on a day-today basis than the shopkeepers in the valleys. If Horne was trying to convert us all to a way of life less likely to harm animals, wouldn't we be more readily convinced to give up eating meat than medical treatment? Meat eating is nothing but an indulgence. Our animal-tested medications, on the other hand, are of proven effectiveness.

Even Linda McCartney, next to Mahatma Gandhi the most famous vegetarian and animal lover of the twentieth century, was fed anticancer drugs as she lay dying-drugs that had been tested on animals. News reports on this are conflicting: either her family did not themselves know that these drugs had been tested on animals, or they knew but were so desperate to see her get better that they did not tell her.

Barry Horne clearly thought he knew what animals were. He was so completely convinced that animals are sentient and worthy of protection that he was willing to lay down his life for them. Linda McCartney clearly also suffered little ambivalence in her attitude toward animals.

Sometimes I wish I could share their certainty.

What are animals-really? What should we make of them? Are they machines: complicated, intricate, beautiful perhaps, but fundamentally mechanical? Or are they something else; something conscious and thoughtful? Are animals possessed of some special spark that sets them off from the mechanical and vegetable worlds? Could they be both at once-conscious machines? Human beings have always lived among other species, and we fret, now perhaps more than ever, over the correct way to deal with them. How can we treat animals appropriately if we don't even know what they are?

As I grew up on the Isle of Wight, the relationship I and my family and friends had with animals expressed the same ambivalence to be found anywhere in the industrial world. We loved our pets, and at some level we knew that the meat on our table had come from animals that lived on the hills around us and that were not dressed up in bows for their birthdays, invited to sleep on their owners' beds, or talked to like bona fide human beings. We lived this dichotomy but, as far as I remember, were little troubled by it. In those days the butchers' shops still had sawdust on the floor and cuts of meat in the window. The meat wasn't packaged on little plastic trays but cut from recognizable limbs of cattle, pigs, and sheep. And yet I don't ever remember being shocked by this. It was just the way of the world.

James Serpell of the University of Pennsylvania expresses the contradictions in our attitudes toward animals very clearly: "At one extreme are the animals we call pets. They make little or no practical or economic contribution to human society, yet we nurture and care for them like our own kith and kin, and display outrage and disgust when they are subjected to ill-treatment. At the other extreme we have animals like the pig on which a major section of our economy depends, supremely useful animals in every respect . . . We pickle its trotters, make black puddings from its blood, sausages from its intestines, and expensive and durable leathergoods from its skin. We even emulsify its thick white fat for the production of ersatz ice-cream. And in return for this outstanding contribution we treat pigs like worthless objects devoid of feelings and sensations."

Can pigs feel? Are they capable of sensations? How would we know? And why doesn't anyone seem to care one way or another? The great mass of pig-eating humanity may not be bothered about the emotional and sensory worlds of the beast that ended up as bacon on the breakfast table, but to me more surprisingly, the animal liberation movement does not seem greatly interested either: The North American Press Office of the Animal Liberation Front issues an annual report on direct action by any individual or organization to liberate animals in the United States or Canada. The 2001 edition-dedicated to Barry Horne-documents the rescue of five thousand animals, not one of them a pig. Why not?

Many people are fascinated and intrigued by animals, and yet very few seem to be aware of the work that has been done in the last fifty years to improve our understanding of animal minds. True, there are a couple of high-profile "discoveries" that everybody knows about. If I had a penny for every time I have been told that chimpanzees are genetically as nearly identical to us as makes no difference and, given appropriate training, can communicate in human language, I would have a great pile of small change. My pockets would also drag if I collected coinage each time I am told that dolphins use an elaborate language among themselves that we are not smart enough to decode. But aside from these high-profile (and highly questionable) discoveries, the public at large just draws a blank. This book is my attempt to fill in some of that blankness, to bring some of the discoveries of modern animal behavior science to a wider audience.

THE SIMILARITY SANDWICH

What I want to do in this book is sweep all the debris of traditional views of animals, now mixed up with mauled science, right off the table and start again-that is, start with the reliable knowledge we have of what animals do. I have often been disappointed by how little scientific work is done on the psychology of animals. Too many psychologists define their sphere of interest as exclusively their own species, just Homo sapiens: How limited! So many exciting things have been discovered about all kinds of species. Just yesterday I was reading about the bolas spider that lures moths by imitating moth pheromones. In a transspecies war of the sexes, the female bolas spider imitates the attractant pheromone of the female moth, so only male moths are attracted to her. The spider has no web: when the moth gets close enough, she hurls a sticky ball (bolas) of silk at the moth to capture him. In America, bolas spiders just swing the bolas back and then out: in Africa and Australia these spiders swirl their bolases around like a lasso before lashing out at the moth. Either way, the moth gets caught, paralyzed, and wrapped in silk.

There is enough science out there to lay the foundation for an objective understanding of animals. I am convinced that we are beginning to know what animals are. And I can tell you up front: animals are not like us. But in many respects they are like us.

Other species are not like us. But they are also like us. I see a "not like us-like us-not like us" sandwich. It looks like this:

The bottom layer is a layer of dissimilarity. Each species on this planet lives in a unique sensory world. The sonar of the hunting bat (and the moth, its prey). The ultraviolet light seen by birds; the infrared of insects. The rich sense of smell dogs enjoy. The electric and magnetic fields to which some fish and a few other animals are sensitive. (The obscure Australian duck-billed platypus can tell if a battery has any current left in it-though there are easier methods of testing batteries.) Birds pick up on changes in air pressure. At this level there is no denying the diversity in the animal kingdom.

The middle layer is a layer of similarity. Here we find basic psychological processes like learning and some kinds of memory, along with simple forms of concept formation, such as identifying objects as being the same or different from other objects and a basic sense of time and number. All of these seem be common to a wide range of species and to operate in similar ways in animals as diverse as chicks and chimpanzees, spiders and squirrel monkeys.

It is not always easy to be sure whether something belongs in the middle layer (similarity) or the bottom layer (dissimilarity) of the sandwich. Studies on reasoning and problem solving, for example, do not seem to reveal many differences between species. But then again, so few species have been studied. And tool use (one of the few activities suggestive of reasoning that can be observed in the field) is definitely very different from species to species (and absent in most). Maybe it isn't a ham sandwich, but one filled with something that can squish into the bread-something like cream cheese? The boundary between the first two levels is not a firm one. My point is that every animal's world is different, just as every animal's lifestyle and niche are different. And yet there are also commonalities in animal minds, because we are all living on the same planet and descended from the same slimy ancestors.

But when we come to the bread on top of the sandwich, we notice something very different. After forty years of trying we can say definitively that no nonhuman primate (or any other species) has ever developed anything equivalent to human language. The hens in the chook house will never "hatch an elaborate plot to escape from the clutches of the menacing Mrs. Tweedy" (to quote from the web site for the movie Chicken Run). Most nonhuman species show very little interest even in imitating each other-let alone communicating with each other and coordinating their activity. Even chimpanzees, though they may recognize themselves in mirrors, are very slow to understand the motives of other individuals. They seem no better able to place themselves imaginatively into the shoes (or paws or hoofs) of another individual than are autistic children. This is a very surprising fact, and one that animal behavior scientists have been reluctant to face up to. There really is a difference between humans and other animals. A pretty big difference. The psychological abilities that make human culture possible-enthusiasm to imitate others, language, and the ability to place oneself imaginatively into another's perspective on events-are almost entirely lacking in any other species. They didn't have to be: we are all related, and we do share a great many psychological qualities with other species. But, as it turns out, we really are different from them in crucial respects.

DARWINIAN ACID

Acknowledging that the similarity sandwich has bread (that is, dissimilarity) on top, that there is something different about us humans, is a hard-wrung admission for me. I am a hard-core Darwinian. I believe that all species on this planet are related, some more closely than others, and that our common stock diverges through a process of selection. Some of us survive and thrive, have children and grandchildren. Others are left on the slag heap of (evolutionary) history. So long as children somewhat resemble their parents, natural selection will shape us to fit our environment.

In a wonderful exploration of Darwin's theory, Daniel Dennett, philosopher at Tufts University, called natural selection universal acid. He meant that it is an idea so powerful that nothing can contain it: it eats through every barrier. I like Dennett's metaphor because it emphasizes how difficult-even dangerous-it can be to work with very powerful ideas.

For example: We share 98.4 percent of our DNA with chimpanzees, and probably even more with bonobos (also known as pygmy chimpanzees). Does this mean that chimpanzees and perhaps other great apes share human self-awareness and are entitled to similar protection under the law as we are? This is what a group calling itself The Great Ape Project claims.

Continues...


Excerpted from Do Animals Think? by Clive D. L. Wynne Excerpted by permission.
All rights reserved. No part of this excerpt may be reproduced or reprinted without permission in writing from the publisher.
Excerpts are provided by Dial-A-Book Inc. solely for the personal use of visitors to this web site.

Table of Contents

1What Are Animals?1
2The Secrets of the Honeybee Machine13
3How Noble in Reason46
4What Is It Like to Be a Bat?84
5Talk to Me106
6The Pigeon That Saved a Battalion139
7Monkey See, Monkey Do?162
8Dolphins Divine195
9Sandwiches to Go222
References245
Acknowledgments261
Index263

What People are Saying About This

Noam Chomsky

Wynne's expert, lucid, sharply argued (and even witty) study provides a wonderful account of what is understood about how animals think and the serious challenges that face scientific study of these fascinating questions. It also offers very reasonable and suggestive thoughts about the place of humans within the rich and complex world of mental achievements and limitations.
Noam Chomsky, Massachusetts Institute of Technology

From the Publisher

"Wynne's expert, lucid, sharply argued (and even witty) study provides a wonderful account of what is understood about how animals think and the serious challenges that face scientific study of these fascinating questions. It also offers very reasonable and suggestive thoughts about the place of humans within the rich and complex world of mental achievements and limitations."—Noam Chomsky, Massachusetts Institute of Technology

"I had more fun reading this book than I have had from any other book in a long time! It is clever, erudite, and accessible."—Jonathan Marks, University of North Carolina, Charlotte

"Clive Wynne has written a vitally important book. A fascinating and authoritative account of the latest research on animal minds, Do Animals Think? is also a much-needed corrective to the half-truths, exaggerations, and fairy tales that have become all too common in this field."—Stephen Budiansky, author of If a Lion Could Talk: Animal Intelligence and the Evolution of Consciousness

Stephen Budiansky

Clive Wynne has written a vitally important book. A fascinating and authoritative account of the latest research on animal minds, Do Animals Think? is also a much-needed corrective to the half-truths, exaggerations, and fairy tales that have become all too common in this field.
Stephen Budiansky, author of "If a Lion Could Talk: Animal Intelligence" and the "Evolution of Consciousness"

Jonathan Marks

I had more fun reading this book than I have had from any other book in a long time! It is clever, erudite, and accessible.
Jonathan Marks, University of North Carolina, Charlotte

Recipe

"Wynne's expert, lucid, sharply argued (and even witty) study provides a wonderful account of what is understood about how animals think and the serious challenges that face scientific study of these fascinating questions. It also offers very reasonable and suggestive thoughts about the place of humans within the rich and complex world of mental achievements and limitations."—Noam Chomsky, Massachusetts Institute of Technology

"I had more fun reading this book than I have had from any other book in a long time! It is clever, erudite, and accessible."—Jonathan Marks, University of North Carolina, Charlotte

"Clive Wynne has written a vitally important book. A fascinating and authoritative account of the latest research on animal minds, Do Animals Think? is also a much-needed corrective to the half-truths, exaggerations, and fairy tales that have become all too common in this field."—Stephen Budiansky, author of If a Lion Could Talk: Animal Intelligence and the Evolution of Consciousness

From the B&N Reads Blog

Customer Reviews